know.txt 3.6 KB

1234567891011121314151617181920212223242526272829303132333435363738394041424344454647484950515253
  1. December 2014I've read Villehardouin's chronicle of the Fourth Crusade at least
  2. two times, maybe three. And yet if I had to write down everything
  3. I remember from it, I doubt it would amount to much more than a
  4. page. Multiply this times several hundred, and I get an uneasy
  5. feeling when I look at my bookshelves. What use is it to read all
  6. these books if I remember so little from them?A few months ago, as I was reading Constance Reid's excellent
  7. biography of Hilbert, I figured out if not the answer to this
  8. question, at least something that made me feel better about it.
  9. She writes:
  10. Hilbert had no patience with mathematical lectures which filled
  11. the students with facts but did not teach them how to frame a
  12. problem and solve it. He often used to tell them that "a perfect
  13. formulation of a problem is already half its solution."
  14. That has always seemed to me an important point, and I was even
  15. more convinced of it after hearing it confirmed by Hilbert.But how had I come to believe in this idea in the first place? A
  16. combination of my own experience and other things I'd read. None
  17. of which I could at that moment remember! And eventually I'd forget
  18. that Hilbert had confirmed it too. But my increased belief in the
  19. importance of this idea would remain something I'd learned from
  20. this book, even after I'd forgotten I'd learned it.Reading and experience train your model of the world. And even if
  21. you forget the experience or what you read, its effect on your model
  22. of the world persists. Your mind is like a compiled program you've
  23. lost the source of. It works, but you don't know why.The place to look for what I learned from Villehardouin's chronicle
  24. is not what I remember from it, but my mental models of the crusades,
  25. Venice, medieval culture, siege warfare, and so on. Which doesn't
  26. mean I couldn't have read more attentively, but at least the harvest
  27. of reading is not so miserably small as it might seem.This is one of those things that seem obvious in retrospect. But
  28. it was a surprise to me and presumably would be to anyone else who
  29. felt uneasy about (apparently) forgetting so much they'd read.Realizing it does more than make you feel a little better about
  30. forgetting, though. There are specific implications.For example, reading and experience are usually "compiled" at the
  31. time they happen, using the state of your brain at that time. The
  32. same book would get compiled differently at different points in
  33. your life. Which means it is very much worth reading important
  34. books multiple times. I always used to feel some misgivings about
  35. rereading books. I unconsciously lumped reading together with work
  36. like carpentry, where having to do something again is a sign you
  37. did it wrong the first time. Whereas now the phrase "already read"
  38. seems almost ill-formed.Intriguingly, this implication isn't limited to books. Technology
  39. will increasingly make it possible to relive our experiences. When
  40. people do that today it's usually to enjoy them again (e.g. when
  41. looking at pictures of a trip) or to find the origin of some bug in
  42. their compiled code (e.g. when Stephen Fry succeeded in remembering
  43. the childhood trauma that prevented him from singing). But as
  44. technologies for recording and playing back your life improve, it
  45. may become common for people to relive experiences without any goal
  46. in mind, simply to learn from them again as one might when rereading
  47. a book.Eventually we may be able not just to play back experiences but
  48. also to index and even edit them. So although not knowing how you
  49. know things may seem part of being human, it may not be.
  50. Thanks to Sam Altman, Jessica Livingston, and Robert Morris for reading
  51. drafts of this.